igapi.blogg.se

Change the name of multiple songs at once
Change the name of multiple songs at once







change the name of multiple songs at once change the name of multiple songs at once

This is because most young birds learn the species-specific songs during the first year of their lives. With some exceptions (e.g., gray catbirds, sedge warblers, brown-headed cowbirds ), birds raised in acoustic isolation during the sensory phase produce atypical songs as adults. Territorial conflicts are energetically costly, and the song of a local population, or dialect, is thus thought to be favored as it can serve as an indication of regional origin, minimizing physically demanding defense measures. When maintaining a breeding territory, familiar neighbors are preferred over newcomers because newcomers, who do not yet have a territory, are more likely to expand their territories than established neighbors. On the other hand, the "song-sharing hypothesis" suggests that male-male competition selects for simpler, more homogenous songs (Beecher & Brenowitz 2005). In fact, in some species females prefer males with large repertoires, and males with larger repertoires have a higher reproductive success (i.e., they produce more offspring) than those with a smaller repertoire (Catchpole & Slater 2008). So what favors large and small repertoire size? According to the "repertoire hypothesis," a large repertoire is thought to be under directional sexual selection (Catchpole & Slater 2008, Beecher & Brenowitz 2005) where the choosy sex (often the females) favors one extreme end of a heritable, sexual trait. As we already learned, variation in repertoire size among species is extensive, and the characteristic of having a repertoire with multiple song types likely evolved more than once in songbirds (MacDougall-Shackleton 1997).









Change the name of multiple songs at once